janetthomas

9780385720274_p0_v1_s260x420
Amatya Sen claims that ‘capabilities’ provide the relevant evaluative space for understanding equality. However Sen’s theory has been criticised on the grounds that it does not provide sufficient specification of which functionings and capabilities are valuable. This essay investigates the capabilities theory with reference to this kind of criticism and finds that, on the basis of its reliance upon universal values of liberty and autonomy,  Martha Nussbaum’s development of capabilities theory could provide a possible framework for specifying valuable functionings and capabilities.
The capabilities approach is dependent upon the evaluation of those particular functionings that are concerned with the value of life. In the study prepared for the United Nations, Amartya Sen put forward the argument that the capability approach is an improvement on other theories of justice, such as utilitarianism, Rawlsian egalitarianism and libertarianism because it uses other informational focuses to consider social advantage (p.30). The main criticism of…

View original post 2,257 more words

janetthomas

download

John Rawls’ “Theory of Justice” (1972) explains how social cooperation needs a system of justice and gives a basic idea of what justice means to the structure of society. Rawls presents the primary concept of justice as being fairness, an abstract extension of the social contract described by such philosophers as Locke, Rousseau and Kant. In Rawls’ theory, however, the social contract is replaced by a concept of an original position which allows for an original agreement on the principles of justice. Rawls contrasts this with the classical utilitarian view (p.3).

Rawls first considers the role of justice. He states that while society is seen as the cooperation of people for mutual advantage it also produces conflict through competing interests. Although social cooperation is the best way for people to better their lives there is a conflict of interests because people dispute inequalities of distribution of the products of society…

View original post 965 more words

janetthomas

???????????????????????????????

Thomas, J.E, (2006), “Humans in Winter”, Encaustic on paper

View original post

janetthomas

MRI
Neuroscience is making advances in mapping our brain. In doing so, it questions our fundamental beliefs about our autonomy within the law. However, to date, it still has not been successful in undermining principles of justice that have underpinned Western legal systems and international treaties for the past centuries. This essay provides a study upon what difference neuroscience makes, or not, to the law.

Neuroscience is an advancement in cognitive studies which has up until a few decades ago relied mostly upon behavioural studies. Presently there are claims made by corporations involved in neuro-technologies that claim to be able to detect deception accurately, and also to assess whether people have tendencies towards criminality. To study the issue of whether neuroscience in this capacity will not make a difference to the law, this essay will study the history of the cognitive sciences in the law, the claims of Greene and…

View original post 2,176 more words

janetthomas

Theory-of-Mind-Mimiandeunice

The capacity to be able to ascertain and attribute mental states to others, and also ourselves, is called mindreading or theory of mind (ToM). It is an essential ability that is needed for proper social interaction and moral competence. Children have usually developed the basics of mindreading by the age of five. They understand that others have desires, emotions and beliefs, and that these may be different from their own. We also tend to understand another’s behaviour through understanding our own, or through introspective reasoning, for example: I am happy, therefore I smile/ They smile, therefore they are happy. These are assumptions of mental states and are evident in children even before they can reason articulately.

An alternative view to the introspective ToM is Gopnick’s and Wellman’s (1992) argument which suggests that the concepts of mindreading and mental states are the premises of a theory. Knowledge of ours and other…

View original post 490 more words

janetthomas

Qumran34s2

To assess the foundations of Christianity and whether it was influenced by the teachings of the community at Qumran this essay will compare and contrast the texts of the Gospels of Matthew and John, the two apostles of Jesus of Nazareth, and the Qumran scrolls, which provide first-hand information on Palestinian-Jewish relationships during the the first century CE[1], as paleographic datings put the Qumran texts into the correct time frame[2]. The Old Testament also provides a background to the ministry of Jesus of Nazareth[3] and the particular Gospels were chosen because of the close relationship Matthew and John had to the life of Jesus. Although the Gospels have been redacted and are more than likely community documents written by more than one person, the texts of Qumran[4] and the books of the Old Testament also have these features. Therefore, through this method I hope…

View original post 1,984 more words

p00w8kpf

Introduction

Theory of Mind relies upon our understanding of relationships and emotions which are essential for human cooperation. There are various methods that the brain uses to enhance the cognitive functions involved in Theory of Mind and this can increase brain plasticity after there has been neuronal damage. Some of these methods involve very ancient techniques to enhance human relationships such as story-telling, art and music. In fact, it is posited by some researchers that the evolution of Theory of Mind was essential to these faculties being developed by humans, and that they still hold the key to brain development but also brain rehabilitation. This paper addresses these claims in three parts- the first part gives an explanation of Theory of Mind; the second gives research evidence as to why it can be useful in rehabilitation after acquired brain injury; and the third explains how it is used in rehabilitation therapies but suggests also that the experience of art forms as a member of an audience can be as important as practicing them.

What is theory of mind?

A basic necessity for human communication is the ability to understand another person’s perspective through the use of one’s imagination. It is a critical tool for maintaining human relationships. Through it we can guess at other people’s motivations, their beliefs, their desires, and describe them on the basis of their intentions. Imagination is the basis of Theory of Mind which is the cognitive tool necessary to create literature, art, drama, humor and music as a means of expression, communication and recognition. Theory of Mind, or folk psychology as it is sometimes known, is based upon the understanding that humans are autonomous agents that have mental states which form beliefs and desires which are the basic motivators of human actions.

Without this cognitive tool it would not be possible to create theories of how other minds might work. Because the mind is opaque, human beings need a model or simulation to understand how another person’s mind is working. Psychologists posit that children develop Theory of Mind at around four years of age. It is at this age that children become self- aware and able to infer what others might be experiencing as well, and this ability requires imagination (Leverage, 2011, p.13). Information gathered from the people we socialize with, such as their behavior and what they speak of, allows us to make inferences about them. From this information we can make a mental model of the person.

It is argued that, like grooming among primates, language developed as human society increased and conversation allowed many more relationships to emerge (ibid, p. 16-17). Using language and the ability to make mental models of others and also mental models of others’ mental models, Theory of Mind has allowed us to be more socially intelligent. The extra neural growth in the brain’s cortex allows humans to maintain several layers of these mental models for different other people. These mental models are called ‘intentional states’, or the states of beliefs, desires, and emotions (ibid, p.18).

An intentional state describes the behavior of a person based upon their knowledge, beliefs and desires. When there is a conflict between reality and a person’s belief, it is the person’s belief that will determine their behavior. The knowledge another person has may be different to my own. For example, I know that a dangerous spider lurks behind that rock and, using that knowledge, I can either warn that person if it is my friend who cannot see it or tell that person it is safe if they are my enemy. In this way I have used Theory of Mind to manipulate the other person’s behavior. The dissemination of false or true information has been predominant in human interaction (Firth & Firth, 2005).

The transmission of knowledge also involves the use of Theory of Mind and, it is contended, is the reason for enabling accumulative cultural knowledge. The maturation of the development of the faculty of Theory of Mind is thought to have coincided with the appearance of homo sapiens around 250-200 thousand years ago. This facilitated an explosion in technical, social and cultural phenomena, such as the earliest forms of fictional art and symbolic adornments during the early Paleolithic era (Livingston 2010, p.95). Theory of Mind allows cultural development faster than genetic evolution can provide. It enables individuals to ‘imagine’ their self acting as another (ibid. p.96). This can be done through sharing emotions which is a unique and crucial element of Theory of Mind (ibid. p.98).

Emotions can be described through symbolism. Symbolic representations arise out of collaborative problem solving. Individuals use these representations for dialogical interaction to produce some form of joint understanding. Symbolism, along with reflective thought, allows a thought to be produced and to be reflected upon. This has two functions: the first is to correctly transmit cultural knowledge, for example a child elicits a thought and an adult will critique it which may force the child to reexamine the thought from a new perspective; the second allows for individual skill development where a person reaches a certain level of skill at a task and is then able to reflect upon the elements that allowed them to achieve it. This allows the creator to act reflexively and hone skills and thoughts by making refinements, adjustments and progressions (ibid. p.99).

Why could Theory of Mind be useful in dealing with brain injuries?

It is posited that the anterior paracingulate cortex along with the superior temporal sulcus, and temporal poles being supportive, are the active regions for Theory of Mind to be mediated (Leverage, 2011, p.3). Progressive enlargement of the cortex, it is argued, has occurred in the evolution of higher order primates because the brain needs to have more neural machinery to contain the extra mental models needed for a larger social group. For humans, language rather than grooming has taken over as the primary means of maintaining relationships in a group. Hominid evolution can be clearly seen through the enlargement of brain-cortex size over a 500 thousand year period. The human cortex became distinctly larger once language-based communication became the primary mode of communication in the group (ibid, p.17). It is argued that laughter and music also added to this increase, along with the functions of Theory of Mind.

Art is an externalization of metaphors and models. Before art emerged around 50 thousand years ago these areas of knowledge within the brain were marked out into plant/food; social world; navigation; toolmaking etc. When art emerged these areas began to overlap and metaphor, or the symbolic significance of an object or idea, became possible (ibid, p.19-20). As the brain used metaphor to communicate, the invention of writing enabled an externalized memory and the possibility of communicating over a distance. Stories became tools for thinking in the social world (ibid, p.20). This can be described as ‘distributed cognition’ and its purpose is increased explanatory power. The psychologist James Werscht gives the example of the story of how his daughter lost her shoes. He tried to help her remember where she left them. Werscht questioned who is doing the remembering? He could not have been, as he did not know where she had left them, and she could not have been remembering as she had forgotten. He argued it was a dyad, two people working as a single cognitive system, to solve a problem on an intermental plane. It is this function that allowed him and his daughter to find her shoes (ibid, p.29).

The strongest engine of the human imagination is this functional capacity for cognitive integration. We don’t just imagine stories or ideas that are the opposite to our immediate environment, we can also blend and connect them to make a mental array (ibid, p.41). Theory of Mind relies upon some basic functions to work such as perspective taking, empathy and generally speculating on the thoughts of others. It is fundamental to social cognition (Nguyen, 2015, p.651). Difficulties with this function can come about through brain damage to the frontal cortex and the region of the temporo-parietal junction (Firth & Firth, 2005).

How could these uses be put into action?

Two regions of the brain consistently ascribed with the abilities to maintain functions associated with Theory of Mind are the medial prefrontal cortex region (paracingulate cortex) and the tempero-parietal junction in the superior temporal sulcus (Firth & Firth, 2005). However, it is also contended that the evidence for a Theory of Mind cognitive network is limited and contentious (Shaafsma et al, 2015, p. 67). This is because it is thought not to be a monolithic process (ibid, p.65). It is proposed that Theory of Mind concepts are broken down into cognitive components which can describe more basic functions. These basic processes can then be mapped onto specific neural processes. No one process can be ascribed to Theory of Mind and every basic task will involve other tasks than those particularly ascribed to Theory of Mind. Theory of Mind emerges from these basic cognitive components because of the shared variance of these components.

Just as humans use the five senses to perceive the physical world around them, they also use several different social-cognitive processes to construct a useful mental view of the world around them and those who inhabit it. A map of such processes can be posited as modular, even though it is by no means certain that the brain is modular in construction. I suggest that a basic map of brain function can be illustrated as in Figure 1. This mind map shows that the basic functions of the brain interact with each other which enhance performance and ability. The map begins with the five senses which transfer information to the thalamus which acts as the basis of the assessment module. It directs the information to various areas of the brain. The hippocampus is the basis of memory and emotion. Interacting with the thalamus it sends messages to the cortex which then directs these messages to the two hemispheres of the brain, the right side concerned with imagination and the left side with ideas. These hemispheres direct the information to the frontal lobe, which is chiefly concerned with action and language. These faculties are all essential for the operation of mind reading skills. Therefore, although there is a good amount of evidence that Theory of Mind is a module that can be damaged, leaving a deficit of social skills, it could be argued that enough stimulus to the brain could allow Theory of Mind to exist in the brain without those particular regions ascribed to it.

Diagram for a modular mind

There is a large amount of credible evidence to show that the brain is plastic and able to transfer function from certain areas to others when there is a necessity to do so. The plasticity of the brain is posited as ‘the ability to make adaptive changes related to the structure and function of the nervous system’ (Zilles, 1992, p.383). The brain can make alterations in neuronal networks which include changes in neuronal connectivity as well as neuronal genesis, and also neurobiochemical changes (Fuchs & Flugge, 2014). Brain training is controversial and experimental studies show that, while effects on trained tasks are consistently reported, there is little evidence for long term effects. However, this area of neuroscience is in great need of larger and more thoroughly designed studies (von Heugten et al, 2016). And it is to this area which this paper seeks to use the basis of Theory of Mind as a rehabilitation design for people with acquired brain injuries.

Tomasello contends that Theory of Mind acts to bring together domain and non-domain specific faculties in the creation of music as it does for language. The motivation of art is the desire to communicate effectively with others; to understand and share the emotions is a crucial aspect of Theory of Mind. Affective engagement can be experienced in a variety of forms, storytelling, music, art, dance and theatre. All of these art forms rely upon cross domain channels of expression (Livingston, 2010, pp.85-6). In particular, the ability of music to change consciousness through engendering affect in individuals can be seen with its universal application in ritual and the marking of large social events, as well as its essential use in contemporary times in art forms such as film. It promotes conjoinment through coordinating the emotions of participants (ibid, 2010, p. 87).

Music therapy is one aspect of rehabilitation that can facilitate the interaction of complex patterns in the brain (Bengtsson, et al 2009; Jennata et al, 2002; Patel, 2003). For example, Broca’s area is used both in the processing of language and music, and also the sequencing of physical movement. Neuroscientists consider that this is because Broca’s area is essential to the sequencing, timing and ‘grammar’ of physical movement, language and music skills (Schlaug, 2008). Research clearly shows that learning music changes the brain with auditory and motor areas growing larger. Therefore, music drives brain plasticity (Pascual Leone, 2001) and can be beneficial for rehabilitation exercises in those with brain injuries.

Art therapy has also been found to be effective in brain rehabilitation practice. Various case studies have looked at people with severe brain damage and its associated functional impairment. A comparison was made before and after the art therapy was undertaken and there appeared improvements in memory, concentration, attention and organization (David, 1999). This was also found in a case study of a chronic stroke patient who had ten weeks of art therapy undertaken twice a week (Kim et al, 2008). The Royal Talbot Rehabilitation Hospital in Melbourne, Australia, uses art therapy as well as more traditional rehabilitation exercises in patients whose speech, attention, problem solving and memory has been affected by an acquired brain injury. It involves the use of various art forms in order to assist expression, trauma, emotions and relationships and differs from traditional art by focusing on process rather than outcome. It also assists in brain plasticity by activating twenty one different components of brain function, thereby exercising neurological pathways that have not been damaged (Harris and Hanson, 2016).

However, most importantly, such therapy cannot sit in a vacuum if it is to be truly successful. Experience is the foundation of brain stimulation and that comes through the senses. To engage in art therapy, one should experience art by visits to galleries, and it is the same for other art forms. To engage in music therapy, the experience of seeing music performed uses all of the senses and stimulates patients’ engagement with their own imagination and ideas. And so it is for theatre and books; going to the theatre is a profound experience which engages both the sensuous and tactile to found and activate ideas, imagination, actions and language for use in therapy outcomes. Therefore, part of ongoing rehabilitation for people who have brain damage should also include the experience of these art forms as well as the opportunity to engage in them.

Conclusion

Imagination is the basis of Theory of Mind in humans. Through its use, executive faculties within the brain are activated, such as intentional states, or our beliefs, desires and emotions. We also use it to attribute such states to others. It can be activated through symbolism and sound, such as in story-telling, plays, art and music. Because the process of Theory of Mind activates neural regions across the brain it is a good contender to promote brain plasticity after damage has occurred, particularly after an acquired brain injury. By appealing to the concept of a modular brain that interacts, the process of stimulating Theory of Mind activity within the brain could be a basis on which to design cognitive therapy for brain injuries. It has been shown that music and art therapy have good outcomes for people with brain injuries. This could be extended, especially with regard to severe brain injury, to include passive as well as active therapy. Therefore, story-telling, music and art are essential to assisting patients with brain injury. Therapy that includes being an audience member for theatre, music, and art are as important for stimulating the brain as participation in these imaginative processes to engage and stimulate the senses fully in experience.

Bibliography:

1. Bengtsson, S.L., Ullen, F., Ehrson, H. H., et al., (2009), Listening to music activates motor and premotor cortices,Cortex 45 (2009), pp. 62–71.

2. David I., (1999), An exploration of the role of art as therapy in rehabilitation from traumatic brain injury, ProQuest Dissertations Publishing.
3. Frith, C.& Frith, U. (2005), Theory of Mind, Current Biology, Volume 15, Issue 17, (2005) pp R644-645, Retrieved from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0960982205009607 on 31 July 2016.

4. Fuchs, E. & Flugge, G. (2014), Adult Neuroplasiticity: More than 40 Years of Research, Neural Plasticity, Volume 2014 , Article ID 541870, 10 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2014/541870.

5. Harris J. and Hansen A., (2005), The role of art therapy in rehabilitation after acquired brain injury, Royal Rehabilitation Centre, Australia, 6th World Congress on Brain Injury, May 6-8, 2005, Melbourne Australia.

6. Van Huegten C.M., Ponds R.W. and Kessels R.P. (2016), Brain training: Hype or hope?, Neuropsychology Rehabilitation, October 2016 Volume 26, Numbers 5-6, pp. 639-644, doi: 10.1080/09602011.2016.1186101.

7. Janata, P., Tillmann, B., and Bharucha, J.J., (2002), Listening to polyphonic music recruits domain-general attention and working memory circuits, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 2, 2002, pp.121-140.
8. Kim S., Kim M., Lee J., and Chun S., (2008), Art therapy outcomes in the rehabilitation treatment of a stroke patient: A Case Report, Art Therapy, Volume 25, Number 3, 2008, pp. 129-133.
9. Leverage, P. (2011), Theory of Mind and Literature, West Lafayette, Indiana, Purdue University Press.

10. Livingstone, S.R. & Thompson, W.F. (2009), The emergence of music from the Theory of Mind, Musicae Scientae, Special Issue (2009-2010), pp. 83-115.

11. Pascual Leone, A., (2001), The brain that plays music and is changed by it, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 930, 2001, pp. 315–329.

12. Patel, A.D., (2003), Language, music, syntax, and the brain, Nature Neuroscience 6, 2003, pp. 674–681.

13. Nguyen Chaplin, L. (2015), Why we think we can’t dance: Theory of Mind and Children’s desire to perform, Child Development, March/April 2015, Volume 86, Number 2, pp. 651-658, University of Illinois, Chicago.

14. Schaafsma, S.M., Pfaff, D., Spunt, R.P., and Adolph, R. (2015), Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, February 2015, Volume 19, Number 2.

15. Schlaug, G., (2008), Music, musicians, and brain plasticity, in S. Hallam, I. Cross, and M. H. Thaut, eds., The Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2008), pp.197–208.

16. Zilles, K., (1992), Neuronal plasticity as an adaptive property of the central nervous system, Annals of Anatomy, 1992, Volume 174, Number 5, pp. 383–391.

 

janetthomas

Afternoon, 2007, oil on canvas

View original post

janetthomas

This essay will explore the similarities and the differences between the texts of the Dead Sea Scrolls, being the Community Rule (1QS) and the Damascus Document (CD), and posit implications for the historical environment of the documents.

 

The 1QS scroll was found in a desert cave at Qumran in 1948. It contains the rules for a religious community that occupied the site from around the late second century BCE to the middle of the first century CE. The CD is a scroll found in the late nineteenth century in a synagogue in Cairo and is a medieval copy of scrolls also found at the Qumran site. The two texts show remarkable similarities in their intent but appear quite different in the theological method and the lifestyle of the two communities that they describe. It will be argued that the two texts have fundamental similarities which show that the CD…

View original post 2,011 more words